Single-subject design

Single-subject design or single-case research design is a research design most often used in applied fields of psychology, education, and human behavior in which the subject serves as his/her own control, rather than using another individual/group. Researchers use single-subject design because these designs are sensitive to individual organism differences vs group designs which are sensitive to averages of groups. Often there will be large numbers of subjects in a research study using single-subject design, however—because the subject serves as their own control, this is still a single-subject design.[1] These designs are used primarily to evaluate the effect of a variety of interventions in applied research.[2]

Requirements of a single-subject design

The following are requirements of single-subject designs:[3]

Phases within single-subject design

It is important that the data are stable (steady trend and low variability) before the researcher moves to the next phase. Single-subject designs produce or approximate three levels of knowledge: (1) descriptive, (2) correlational, and (3) causal.[4]

Flexibility of the design

Single-subject designs are preferred because they are highly flexible and highlight individual differences in response to intervention effects.[5] In general, single-subject designs have been shown to reduce interpretation bias for counselors when doing therapy.[6]

Interpretation of data

In order to determine the effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable, the researcher will graph the data collected and visually inspect the differences between phases. If there is a clear distinction between baseline and intervention, and then the data returns to the same trends/level during reversal, a functional relation between the variables is inferred.[7] Sometimes, visual inspection of the data demonstrates results that statistical tests fail to find.[8][9]

Researchers utilizing single-subject design begin with graphic analysis. During the baseline, data are repeatedly collected and then graphed on the behavior of interest. This provides a visual representation of the subject's behavior before application of the intervention. It is critical that several (three to five is often recommended[10]) data points are collected during baseline to allow the researcher to describe the effects on the target behavior during intervention.

In interpreting, the general strategy of all single-subject research is to use the subject as their own control. Experimental logic argues that the subject's baseline behavior would match its behavior in the intervention phase unless the intervention does something to change it. This logic then holds to rule out confound, one needs to replicate. It is the within-subject replication and allows for the determination of functional relationships. Thus the goal is:

Limitations of single-subject design

Research designs are traditionally preplanned so that most of the details about to whom and when the intervention will be introduced are decided prior to the beginning of the study. However, in single-subject designs, these decisions are often made as the data are collected.[11] In addition, there are no widely agreed-upon rules for altering phases, so—this could lead to conflicting ideas as to how a research experiment should be conducted in single-subject design.

The major criticism of single-subject designs are:

History

Historically, single-subject designs have been closely tied to the experimental analysis of behavior and applied behavior analysis.[12]

See also

References

  1. Cooper, J.O.; Heron, T.E.; Heward, W.L. (2007). Applied Behavior Analysis (2nd ed.). Prentice Hall. ISBN 0-13-142113-1.
  2. Kazdin p. 191
  3. Kazdin, pp. 103–10
  4. Tripodi, T. (1998). A Primer on Single-Subject Design for Clinical Social Workers. Washington, DC: National Association of Social Workers (NASW) Press
  5. Thompson, C.K. (1986). Flexibility of Single-subject Experimental Designs. Part III: Using Flexibility to Design or Modify Experiments. The Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51(3), 214–25
  6. Moran, D.J. & Tai, W. (2001). Reducing Biases in Clinical Judgment with Single-Subject Treatment Design. The Behavior Analyst Today, 2(3), 196–206 BAO
  7. Backman, C.L. & Harris, S.R. (1999). Case Studies, Single-Subject Research, and N of 1 Randomized Trials. Comparisons and Contrasts. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 78(2), 170–6.
  8. Bobrovitz, C.D. & Ottenbacher, K.J. (1998). Comparison of Visual Inspection and Statistical Analysis of Single-Subject Data in Rehabilitation Research. Journal of Engineering and Applied Science, 77(2), 94–102.
  9. Nishith, P.; Hearst, D.E.; Mueser, K.T. & Foa, E. (1995). PTSD and Major Depression: Methodological and Treatment Considerations in a Single-Case Design. Behavior Therapy, 26(2), 297–9
  10. Alberto, P.A. & Troutman, A.C. (2006). Applied behavior analysis for teachers (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc.
  11. Kazdin, p. 284
  12. Kazdin, p. 291

Further reading

This article is issued from Wikipedia - version of the 8/7/2016. The text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution/Share Alike but additional terms may apply for the media files.